Anxious People Are More Likely to Walk Left When Blindfolded

By Elizabeth Preston | January 22, 2016 2:08 pm

7969156134_b8dc6b8075_z

Could you walk in a perfectly straight line while blindfolded? It’s lucky this skill isn’t demanded often, because people aren’t very good at it. Most veer a little to one side. And the direction you err in may be tied to your personality. People with more anxious personalities, for example, tend to drift left. The insight could help scientists learn where these personality traits reside in the brain.

(If you’re thinking that being asked to walk around blindfolded might make you anxious, yes—and hold that thought.)

Mario Weick, a psychologist at the University of Kent in the UK, and his colleagues were interested in two clusters of emotions and motivations that can be used to describe someone’s personality. One cluster, which psychologists call the “behavioral approach system” (BAS), involves positive emotions and pleasure seeking. The other cluster is the “behavioral inhibition system” (BIS); this involves anxiety and inhibition. Weick says the two systems work together to balance our desires and motivations. A person might score especially high in one category or the other; someone with a high BIS score would be very cautious and worried, while someone high in BAS would be driven and joyful. But you can also score high in both areas.

Earlier studies have linked BAS, the more positive personality cluster, to activity in the left side of the brain. There have been hints that in certain circumstances, this activity shifts a person’s attention to the right. (The left side of your brain controls the right side of your body, and vice versa.)

Weick wanted to find out whether this effect would show up in a blindfolded person trying to walk in a straight line. He gathered 78 students, mostly female. Subjects filled out a personality questionnaire that would give them a score for BIS and BAS. Then they stood in an ordinary room and looked at an X on the floor, six meters in front of them. After donning “blindfold goggles” that blocked their vision, they attempted to walk in a straight line toward the X.

At the end of each walk, an experimenter stopped the subjects, walked them back to the start, and let them take off the goggles and look back at the X. Then subjects walked again. Each person took 20 blind walks while a motion-capture camera system recorded his or her trajectory. (The subjects wore a marker on top of their heads for the cameras to follow.)

“It was surprising for us how difficult it was for people to walk straight,” Weick says. Despite their struggles, subjects seemed to enjoy the challenge. The greatest ordeal may have been for the research assistant who guided subjects back and forth, and ended up covering more than 10 miles during the experiment.

It was rare for someone to end up smack on the X. Most people drifted, Weick says, sometimes right and sometimes left. When the researchers looked at all these meandering trajectories together, though, they saw some trends. More walks drifted left than right (918 compared to 621). Leftward drifts were slightly more pronounced, ending up about 34 centimeters from the target on average (rightward drifts averaged about 30 centimeters). More of the subjects ended a majority of their walks on the left (48, as opposed to just 23 who ended most walks to the right). Seven people erred left on every walk, while only one person went consistently right.

Finally, the researchers compared people’s walking results to their personality questionnaires. They saw that people who scored high for BIS had the greatest leftward bias. In other words, more anxious people were more likely to go left. Among people who scored especially low for BIS, those who also had a high BAS score (driven, pleasure-seeking) tended to go right.

This doesn’t mean your worry-wart personality has you wandering leftward throughout your life. Rather, Weick thinks the slightly scary challenge of walking blindfolded helped to activate the anxious trait cluster in people who were inherently biased that way. “We created the circumstances for differences in dispositions to emerge,” he says.

Although watching people’s internal compasses go crooked is a fun trick, Weick says it’s also “an important finding.” It suggests that the cautious, worrying traits of BIS are tied to activation in the right side of the brain. This fits with previous research, Weick says. For scientists, it’s one more tiny, wobbling, uncertain step toward understanding how the brain works.
Image: by Adam Cohn (via Flickr)

Weick M, Allen JA, Vasiljevic M, & Yao B (2016). Walking blindfolded unveils unique contributions of behavioural approach and inhibition to lateral spatial bias. Cognition, 147, 106-12 PMID: 26658021

MORE ABOUT: Psychology
ADVERTISEMENT
  • ckc_not_kc

    …any allowance for handedness? As a righty, if I were walking blind, I would tend to keep my right side forward (defence – probably leading to a leftward circle).

    • Elizabeth Preston

      All the subjects were right-handed.

      • NicholB

        clearly would be interesting to do an experiment with lefties too..

  • Sandra Eagle

    I believe we are systems of energy, and to be well we need to know
    how to work with energy. It’s not enough to talk about issues and leave
    the energy of the issue still wired in the body mechanizations.

    The techniques I use help you learn how to let the energy of an
    emotion ‘complete’ itself in safety, and result in greater
    self-awareness, clarity, and possibility.

    Email me or call me at 203-629-3545

    http://www.sandraeagle.com/mind-body-healing/

NEW ON DISCOVER
OPEN
CITIZEN SCIENCE
ADVERTISEMENT

Inkfish

Like the wily and many-armed cephalopod, Inkfish reaches into the far corners of science news and brings you back surprises (and the occasional sea creature). The ink is virtual but the research is real.

About Elizabeth Preston

Elizabeth Preston is a science writer whose articles have appeared in publications including Slate, Nautilus, and National Geographic. She's also the former editor of the children's science magazine Muse, where she still writes in the voice of a know-it-all bovine. She lives in Massachusetts. Read more and see her other writing here.

ADVERTISEMENT

See More

@Inkfish on Twitter

ADVERTISEMENT

Discover's Newsletter

Sign up to get the latest science news delivered weekly right to your inbox!

Collapse bottom bar
+