Why Did Consciousness Evolve, and How Can We Modify It, Pt. II: The Supremacy of Vision

By Malcolm MacIver | May 23, 2011 12:48 am

Update 8/8/11: The conversation continues in Part III here.

I’m back after a hiatus of a few weeks to catch up on some stuff in the lab and the waning weeks of spring quarter teaching here at Northwestern. In my last post, I put forward an idea about why consciousness– defined in a narrow way as “contemplation of plans” (after Bridgeman)–evolved, and used this idea to suggest some ways we might improve our consciousness in the future through augmentation technology.

Here’s a quick  review: Back in our watery days as fish (roughly, 350 million years ago) we were in an environment that was not friendly to sensing things far away. This is because of a hard fact about light in water, which is that our ability to see things at a far distance is drastically compromised by attenuation and scattering of light in water. A useful figure of merit is “attenuation length,” which in water is tens of meters for light, while in air it is tens of ten thousand meters. This is in perfectly clear water –add a bit of algae or other kinds of microorganisms and it goes down dramatically. Roughly speaking, vision in water is similar to driving a car in a fog. Since you’re not seeing very far out, the idea I’ve proposed goes, there is less of an advantage to planning over the space you can sense. On land, you can see a lot further out. Now, if a chance set of mutations gives you the ability to contemplate more than one possible future path through the space ahead, then that mutation is more likely to be selected for.

Over at Cosmic Variance, Sean Carroll wrote a great summary of my post. Between my original post and his, many insightful questions and problems were raised by thoughtful readers.

In the interest of both responding to your comments and encouraging more insightful feedback, I’ll have a couple of further posts on this idea that will explore some of the recurring themes that have cropped up in the comments.

Today, since many commenters raised doubts about my claim that vision on land was key – raising the long distance sensory capabilities of our sense of smell, and hearing, among other points – I thought I’d start with a review of why, among biological senses, only vision (and, to a more limited degree echolocation) is capable of giving access to the detail that could be necessary to having multiple future paths to plan over. Are the other types of sensing that you’ve raised as important as sight?

Having the kind of overview needed for real-time planning of a path to a goal – at least an unpredictable, moving goal like prey – requires being able to access detail over a large amount of space relative to where you are moving in your immediate future.  I’ll show why the only types of biological sensing capable of providing this sort of broad overview to animals are sight and echolocation, and why sight is easily the more powerful of the two.

Two important factors determine one’s ability to sense from a distance: resolution (the minimum size an object or a feature can be before you can no longer distinguish it), and range (how far away an object can be detected). Given our particular terrestrial environment, sight wins out over all other types of sensing on both counts.

First, a little bit on our yardsticks. Range designates the maximum typical distances that something is sensed. Resolution is fairly intuitive these days, since many of us have had the experience of working with some image we’ve grabbed from the internet with a resolution that is too low for our needs. You can measure it in a variety of ways, such as how many pixels can be resolved or displayed in a given unit of length. The new iPhone’s “retina” display has a resolution of 300 pixels per inch, for example, and as the publicity has suggested, this is similar to the resolving power of our eyes when the display is held at typical viewing distances.

For biological senses, resolution is partially set by how densely packed the sensory receptors are. For visual systems, the packing density in the fovea (for animals that have them), at the central part of the retina, is extremely high, and the density rapidly diminishes away from the fovea.

But there is another constraint, besides how closely spaced the sensory receptors are: the wavelength of the energy you are sensing the world with. As a first approximation, you cannot resolve objects below the wavelength of the energy you are sensing with.  This is true whether you are sensing the consequence of probing the environment with that energy, as in the case of bats and their echolocation, or just passively absorbing the energy emitted by some external object, such as an object reflecting sunlight into your visual system. In the case of vision, the wavelengths are small compared to the packing density of our sensory receptors, so we don’t notice this issue. In the case of probing with sound using an artificial sense (for humans), such as ultrasound, or in the case of echolocation for bats and dolphins, the resolution limits imposed by the energy become more constraining. At 80,000 cycles per second (what some bats use, and four times higher than we can hear), resolution is about one quarter of a centimeter. Dolphins emit at somewhat higher frequencies, but because sound goes about four times faster in water than in air, they end up with a resolution of about 1 centimeter.

With that background on range and resolution, we can ask “what senses provide detailed overviews at far distances (say, at least 100 times longer than your body)?”

Let’s go through some of the biological possibilities: hearing; echolocation, also referred to as sonar (which also involves hearing, but at a much higher frequency, and includes the generation of an echolocation beam); touch; taste; smell; flow sensing (in science referred to as the “mechanosensory lateral line”); sensing of weak electric fields, called “electrosense”; active electrical sensing, called “electrolocation” (similar to normal electrosense, but like echolocation, includes not only perception of electric fields, but generation of them as well—so hearing is to echolocation what electrosense is to electrolocation); magnetosense, the ability to sense Earth’s magnetic field; vision (all types, including polarized light, and ultraviolet). For simplicity, I will consider these one at a time, although in many biological situations, multiple senses would be combined.

Passive hearing: sound can travel a long distance before it can no longer be heard. Underwater, it can travel even further. But there is a problem: hearing can tell you something out there is producing sound (like a screeching animal), but it cannot tell you anything about all the things that are not producing sounds, like the quietly resting boulders nearby the screeching animal or the ferns silently bending in the wind across the stream from said animal. This is in great contrast with vision in daylight: everything that reflects light, which is basically everything, can be seen.

As a consequence, when you hear something, you can get a sense of the direction of the object producing sound, and an estimate of distance. So you can get closer to the thing that produces the sound, but using sound alone, it’s challenging to be clever about how you get closer, since you don’t know anything about the stuff in between you and the thing generating sound (again, we are taking these senses one at a time). If you’ve ever played the Hot and Cold game as a kid, this is similar: the sound gives you enough information to tell if you’re getting hot or cold (approaching or moving away), plus some sense of distance and what kind of object is making the sound.

Active hearing (echolocation, or sonar): echolocation has many of the benefits of vision, but without requiring light. Bats and dolphins generate echolocation pulses which travel out and then return after being reflected by nearby objects. By moving the parts of their body that generate the echolocation pulse (mouth or nose), they can “scan” their environment. However, both resolution and range is significantly worse than in the case of vision, at least on land. We already went through resolution limits of echolocation. In terms of the range of echolocation, in water it is quite good – up to one hundred meters for the kinds of objects dolphins hunt for  — far better than vision in water. It’s interesting that a mammal, that may have been used to large visual ranges on land prior to going back to the ocean, came up with a style of sensing that gives you the best long distance sensing in water. Due to more rapid attenuation of high frequencies in air, bats have a shorter range – on the order a few meters for their prey.

The primary reason for the short range of echolocation systems is that their probe signal falls off with the fourth power of distance. This means that in order to double the range of an echolocation system, you need 16 times more power. Obtaining large ranges with echolocation, therefore, runs into energy consumption issues, and limits to the loudness of sounds that can be generated before damage to tissue ensues.

Touch/taste: This one is easy. While for small insects and rodents, touch appendages can reach out for a good fraction of body length, one body length is about the maximum for the length of things like whiskers and antennae before they become unwieldy. Taste sensors are on the body surface or on things like the tongue, so like touch, isn’t great for sensing at a distance.

Smell: Like passive hearing, the sense of smell can have fantastic range (sharks can smell injured prey from 5 km; male moths can find female moths at up to 10 km). But once again, it only tells you about things emitting odors. This allows you to approach them (if you are lucky with respect to environmental conditions), but you can’t use smell for a detailed overview of the space ahead. It’s fun imagining what would be needed in order to have smell work this way. Every object would need to be emitting a distinct odor, and downstream, these odors would have to stay relatively separated. Then, by scanning your nose through the odor array, you might be able to obtain an “olfactograph” of the space ahead!

Flow sensing: Fish and some other aquatic animals possess special sense organs for detecting flows due to the movement of other animals. This can guide predatory strikes. Seals have been demonstrated to be able to follow flows made by fish after some time has elapsed. In general, however, flow sensing is very “near field”, operating on the range of a body length or two at most.

Passive electrosense. Because all animals in water generate a weak bioelectric field, the ability to detect these fields evolved very early in the history of animals. They are found, for example, in the most ancient vertebrate that still exists, the lamprey (so old it doesn’t even have a jaw). Many other aquatic animals have them as well, such as sharks. The detection of external bioelectric fields occurs at very near range, about a body length or two.

Active electrosense (electrolocation). In active electrical sensing (also called electrolocation), an animal detects how its environment is modulating a self-generated weak electric field. In my doctoral work, I showed that it is effective at less than a body length for prey-like objects, and perhaps a few body lengths for larger objects. Like echolocation, the fall off of active electrosense is with the fourth power of distance, so it rapidly becomes prohibitive to sense at a distance.

Magnetic field sensing: Certain animals have been shown to detect the direction of Earth’s magnetic field. This is very useful for navigation. It should be clear, however, that it will not, in any circumstance, provide a detailed overview of the space ahead.

Vision: Given our relatively transparent environment, illuminated for at least a portion of the day with loads of light from the sun (about a thousand watts of light per square meter on a clear day at noon, a typical “radiant flux density” at the surface of Earth), vision reigns king as a system for imaging. It’s true that some land environments are dense enough to make vision nearly as short as it is in water – but in places like tidal flats, savannah, and prairie, being able to see far ahead pays big dividends.

Because of the high velocity of the electromagnetic radiation vision uses, the resolution limit for visible light is much, much smaller than our ability to perceive, because the distance between our sensory organs for light is quite large compared to the wavelength of light (for example 500 billionths of a meter is one of the wavelengths we see with). As a consequence, as the distance between receptors of the eye has decreased, and our optical abilities along with it, we are able to resolve a sixtieth of one degree with our visual systems. That means we can see a rabbit at a bit over half a mile, an astonishing capability compared to how far out our water-based ancestors could sense.

In contrast, as my original post mentioned, because of the “attenuation length” of light in water, the distance at which 63% of the light from an object is absorbed by the water, is on the order of tens of meters in perfectly clear water. So light from the sun has to go down into water, thereby losing 63% of its intensity after tens of meters – and then reflect off an object, and get to your eye, again losing 63% of its intensity in some tens of meters. In costal waters or anywhere the water is a bit cloudy with phytoplankton or algae, attenuation length is ten times less – going down to meters. No matter what you do with your sensors and optics, this is going to result in significantly diminishing returns to see things further away.

On land, the attenuation length for light in air is on the order of 100 km. This is similar to the attenuation length of sound in water, which is why dolphins and whales do so well with echolocation underwater (but still, for dolphins only on the order of 100 meters for prey-sized objects).

That finishes our survey of what senses are good for quickly accessing points in a big amount of space. To sum up: to sense something means you need to detect energy emanating from the object. Some things, like sounds or odors emitted by animals or environmental phenomena, are sparsely distributed (not every point in your surroundings is emitting the energy), and this feature enables us to find the croaking frog or cracking branch.

But, in such situations, because our ability to sense these objects depends to some extent on the surrounding objects NOT emitting any such energy, it is not possible to get a detailed point by point sensation of a large amount of space. In contrast, with vision, echolocation, and active electrosense, energy is delivered to all objects of interest. So, you can sense them, whether or not they emit any kind of energy on their own. As such, only these senses (and similar ones) have the capacity to provide detailed point-by-point overviews. Of these, vision on land is by far the most powerful, in part just because there is an intense amount of energy being delivered by our Sun for at least a portion of the day, and easily delivered by artificial means otherwise; and in part, because the short wavelength means that vision systems can perceive with unparalleled acuity.

In the next post, I’ll explore the connection between having a big amount of space at hand, and planning to an unpredictable, moving goal, like another animal you’re hoping to dine on. I’ll argue that such planning requires you to have a big chunk of space at the beck and call of your sensory system, relative to the space you’re about to move into.

Image by Malcolm A. MacIver

Correction: In the original post I stated “Dolphins emit at somewhat higher frequencies, but because light goes about four times faster in water than in air, they end up with a resolution of about 1 centimeter.” Thanks to @Kees for pointing out my mistake – I meant that sound goes four times faster in water.

CATEGORIZED UNDER: Biology, Mind & Brain, Neuroscience

Comments (13)

  1. Kees

    Dolphins emit at somewhat higher frequencies, but because light goes about four times faster in water than in air, they end up with a resolution of about 1 centimeter.

    I think you meant to say: … sound goes about …

  2. gc

    Antonio Damasio believes that consciousness is a feeling …
    http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Antonio_Damasio

  3. Aaron

    I flat out LOVE this post.

    As Kees notes there is that mistake and a few other errors you might want to go back and weed out, but it was an extremely good read overall.

    Thanks!

  4. Malcolm MacIver

    @Kees – thanks for pointing out this mistake. Fixed.

  5. jld

    Some humans are quite good at echolocation too…

  6. yogi-one

    Interesting definition of consciousness: “the ability to plan”.

  7. A great post, lots for my consciousness to assimilate through mind and my many senses.

  8. Matthew Saunders

    Coolies post; this reminds me of some of the good stuff of College, where we’d go on riffs on things like “Ok. You’re a butterfly, what is it like?” :)

    So, contemplation of plans…hmm…could you see yourself doing a post on why some matter would have contemplation of plans? What advantages would it give them?

  9. Consciousness evolved in human spices only because our brain developed more speedily than other animal.Main reason for man development is we started to walk on lag our hand are free.Our consciousness developed rapidly because our fear of death and survival instinct developed rapidly.This survival instinct made us live in group , so our language developed for communication.Real reason of involvement of consciousness is to overcome fear of death.

  10. Dante

    Even though I agree with sight being a good (not the supreme) sense, I’m still not convinced that we can define consciousness. In the definition given, your argument works.

  11. This is a very compelling argument for the emergence of plan-making. But, as Dante says, this is not a compelling argument for the emergence of “consciousness.” I lean toward the idea that consciousness is a feeling, or a collection of feelings, or a state of being—what it’s like to be _____. There is something that it is like to be a fish, therefore fish are conscious.

    But what I like about the focus on plan-making is that it provides a transition point at which an organism has a kind of consciousness that is much more morally significant. In college I became a vegetarian for a variety of reasons, not least of which was the idea that animals are conscious (by my definition) and that consciousness is morally relevant. But I’ve since repented on that point, recognizing that not all consciousnesses are created equal (and for those interested in my diet, I still eat mostly-vegetarian with the occasional bite or two of delicious, tasty animal flesh as a rare treat). Some consciousnesses are much more morally significant than others, IMO, and plan-making seems to be a big part of my intuition here.

    (And before anyone brings it up, yes, this means that human embryos, newborns, and even the severely developmentally disabled may not have morally significant consciousnesses. But there are plenty of other criteria by which they are still deserving of moral consideration, not least of which is that they are human and we ought to take care of our own. Moving on.)

    The argument from your first post that shared awareness will make us more moral is a tricky place to tread. On the one hand, people will pick up on the Borg thing, and connect it to 1984 and groupthink, and the next thing you know you’re being called a Nazi. But while it’s true that mob mentality and propaganda emphasizing group loyalty can lead us down the worst paths humans are capable of treading, that is not the necessary outcome of a breakdown of individuality. Buddhism and other peaceful, introspective doctrines teach that individuality is an illusion and encourage rejection of the self in favor of a unifying worldview in which consciousness is seen as a pervasive and fundamental part of all existence.

    (I’m not saying Buddhists are right. But I am saying that, despite systematically dismantling the “self”, they’re not Nazis.)

    So don’t get too optimistic that shared awareness is a moral panacea. It could go either way—most likely both. Sense-sharing technology would certainly have applications for disaster relief and other “good” causes. But let’s be honest: the moment we invent that stuff, it’s going to be used for porn.

  12. Andy

    In a response to the first post, I pointed out that equating consciousness with the ability to plan, or entertain multiple scenarios before acting, is problematic. There is no reason in principle why an unconscious organism could not plan, indeed, in theory why an organism just like a human being could not be entirely unconscious (the philosophical zombie).

    I will add here that I do agree very much with the claim that vision is the most powerful sense, and that there are good reasons for thinking its evolution would be much more advantageous on land. But rather than equate this with consciousness, how about with dimensionality? That is, vision is closely associated with the ability to experience (assuming the organism is conscious) or sense (without making any assumptions about consciousness) three dimensional space.

    Other senses are generally associated with the experience of fewer spatial dimensions. Consider smell or chemosensitivity. You note that passive sensation of chemicals and some other stimuli is like a game of hot or cold. A more precise way of putting this is to say that it’s associated with one dimensional experience. When an organism (or even a chemosensitive bacterium) moves towards a chemical-emitting source, it needs to sense only a single dimension of space: an axis formed by the source at one end and itself at the other. As the stimulus, such as concentration of chemical substance in the water, becomes greater, it is closer to the source, and there is a relationship, albeit not a linear one, between concentration and distance. In fact, the most primitive organisms (such as various phyla of worms) sense the world largely in this fashion.

    Somewhat more evolved organisms, such as insects and other arthropods, also make extensive use of chemical signals (pheromones, for example), but generally live on land and also have visual systems. These organisms might be said to be in a between-state where their experience of the environment is largely two-dimensional. This has been shown, for example, in forager ants, which measure the two-dimensional distance between their nest and a food source–even when they must climb intervening hills–and the praying mantis, which will attack cardboard rectangles if they have two-dimensional relationships similar to those of normal prey.

    Full three-dimensional experience appears to have evolved in the highest invertebrates–flying insects such as bees, and cephalopods like the octopus–and is present in probably all vertebrates. Almost all of these organisms rely heavily on vision, though of course may make use of other senses as well.

  13. Malcolm MacIver

    @Andy – in my argument, nothing turns on three-dimensionality. A cyclops would be fine, so long as the eye sees far enough out, and the brain it is part of can use monocular cues (like occlusion) to make guesses about depth.

NEW ON DISCOVER
OPEN
CITIZEN SCIENCE
ADVERTISEMENT

Discover's Newsletter

Sign up to get the latest science news delivered weekly right to your inbox!

ADVERTISEMENT

See More

ADVERTISEMENT
Collapse bottom bar
+

Login to your Account

X
E-mail address:
Password:
Remember me
Forgot your password?
No problem. Click here to have it e-mailed to you.

Not Registered Yet?

Register now for FREE. Registration only takes a few minutes to complete. Register now »